International Archives of the Photogrammetry, Remote Sensing and Spatial Information Sciences, Vol XXXV, Part B5. Istanbul 2004
the area of rear triangle is Wev /(2W — 2e).
Equations (1) and (2) define the movement boundary for
operators during autostereoscopic measurement. Within this
boundary, operators can move their heads forward, backward,
upward, and downward and still perceive correct 3D images.
The free space in which the operators can move depends on the
monitor size, the viewing distance and eye base. It is also
shown that the front range f, is slightly smaller than the rear
range 7, In our DTI monitor, the front range and rear range are
o n c 5
respectively estimated as 10.8 and 14.2 cm from nominal
viewing line, yielding totally 25 cm continuous range for a
viewer to adjust his or her position in the direction
perpendicular to the screen plane. This study also shows that
the trapezoids of all viewing zones have the same volume.
For the DTI monitor in our study, the number of viewing zone
IS 7. A viewer can obtain stereo effect in seven locations by
adjusting his or her position laterally. It should be noted that
these are the locations that perfect stereo effect is ensured. In
fact, a viewer can move his or her head outside the range
defined by the monitor width. Therefore, there are practically
more than 7 zones where viewers can receive stereo effect.
However, since the viewing direction is not right perpendicular
to the monitor in this situation, the magnitude of the light
transmitted to the viewer's eyes is considerably reduced. As a
result of this, the stereo view will become darker while the
viewer positions are away from the screen center.
3.2 Perceived Depth
Human eyes are able to perceive depth to view 3D objects and
distinguish the distance correctly (Jones et al, 2001). The
perceived depth of an autostereoscopic mapping system
determines the resolution for the object elevation to be
measured. Hence the geometry of perceived depth should be
discussed. The perceived depth is caused by horizontal parallax,
which is the distance between corresponding points in two
different images. When the correct stereo view is observed, the
two optical axes intersect in front of the display plane. The
image presented to the right eye of the observer appears to the
left, and the left image presented to the left eye will appear to
the right. The perceived depth appears in the front of display
plane. The geometry is shown in Figure 4.
v
><
*: Image point
+ e —
Y
Z
Figure 4. Perceived depth in front of the display plane.
In Figure 4, "' denotes the distance between display and
viewer's eyes; p is represented the parallax of corresponding
points on two images. The perceived depth ^? is the offset
term ahead the display to the image point fused by viewer's
d a ae plz, =elv'—z)
eyes. The geometry relation is given as ; s
Thus the relationship between the perceived depth ^^ and the
viewing distance can be written as
1
Lir pv
= >
exp (3)
If we treat e and p as constants, the perceived depth is linearly
proportion to the viewing distance. From Eq. (3) when viewer's
head moves toward to the display plane, the perceived depth
becomes shorter; viewer's head moves away from the display
plane, the perceived depth becomes longer from the display
plane. The change of perceived depth inside the viewing zone
is also linearly proportion to the change of viewing distance
p
Az =
p e + p
Av'
. According to the geometry of viewing zone,
the limitation of viewer's head moving boundary inside the
Av' = 2ev(W + e)/[W(W +2e)]
viewing zone is in xz-plane.
Therefore, the maximum change of perceived depth inside the
viewing zone is
dey = Xo QevW vere gp
: (4)
If the e and viewing distance V' are treated as constants, the
differentiation of Eq. (3) with respect to the desired variable p
directly yields
dz = Xe 4
?^ (e py (5)
Eq. (5) is the relationship between the perceived depth
difference and the horizontal parallax difference. Assuming
e=65em D = 1 to 25 cm, we can plot Eq. (5) to Figure 5 for
different viewing distances.
dz
p (cm)
Figure 5. Ratio of perceived depth difference and parallax
difference.
As is shown, the perceived depth difference is dependent on the
parallax difference of the images. The relationship is not linear.
In general, z, is inversely proportional to the horizontal parallax;
the perceived depth is directly proportional to the viewing
distance. The perceived depth difference dz, is amplified under
autostereoscopic mode because the parallax p is small
comparing to the view distance. This is a good property of
autostereoscopic monitor. This means the horizontal parallax
difference dp can be more apparently reflected in the change of
perceived depth dz,.
The
autc
2D
a sc
resc
autc
cha
cha
DT
200
In
me:
aut
Mic
Inte
two
fing
me:
Th
cre
of
dis
hoi
vel
twi
ste
twi
ess
the
the
co
int
ide
ha